Gap Analysis/Dyslexia

From Cognitive Accessibility Task Force

Deprecated content. This is now part of the Research Module Working Draft

--

Dyslexia User Group Research Module

Note: This page is out of date. This page has been ported to the github version. If you have edits for this page please send them to the list or to Michael and Lisa.

NOTE

We are using this wiki page (and sub pages) as a white board for building a document and collecting notes. Information on this page should NOT be quoted.

Description

Dyslexia is a syndrome best known for its affect on the development of literacy and language related skills. There are a number of different definitions and descriptions of dyslexia. The syndrome of dyslexia is now widely recognized as being a specific learning disability of neurological origin that does not imply low intelligence or poor educational potential, and which is independent of race and social background.

Cognitive Functions

This section is a technical reference. Jump to the next section on #Symptoms for more practical information.

Overview: Mainstream credible research in behavioral neurology agrees that Dyslexia is a consequence of an altered neural substrate, in the various regions of the brain which are responsible for the reading process. fMRI scans (18, 19) have show that different subgroups of dyslexia exhibit under activity in areas such as:

  • V5/MT (BA Area 19) - resulting in visual discriminatory problems (9, 15), possibly disturbing magnocellular function 41
  • auditory cortex (BA areas 41 and 42 ) - resulting in low auditory discrimination skills (32),
  • superior temporal gyrus (BA Area 22) Wernike's area and striate cortex or V1(Area 17)- resulting in a phoneme processing problem (5, 22, 23 ), and pattern recognition.
  • the angular gyrus (Area 39) in the inferior parietal lobule - coursing poor cross modal associations (22, 24, 28, 30).

Other studies #42 using PET have shown less activation than the controls in left inferior frontal gyrus (BA areas 45/44/47/9), left inferior parietal lobule (BA area 40), left inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus (BA areas 20/37) and left middle temporal gyrus (BA area 21). There are also studies with different approaches such as identifying ectopias clustered round the left temporoparietal language areas. #44

The different schools of research have championed the different neurological bases of dyslexia, and its resulting subgroups of dyslexia.


Auditory Discrimination

(Main research - see Tallal et all (32)) This body of research has shown that many dyslexics have defects in the left auditory cortex. The auditory cortex is responsible for sound naming and identification and temporal processing (such as interval, duration, and motion discrimination).

Note that dyslexia does not affect hearing, but the identification and differentiation of sounds.

Visual Recognition Skills

(Main school of research Livingstone (1993) and Martin and Lovegrove 1988) )(see 9, 15) Dyslexics have reduced synaptic activity in the V5 area (also known as visual area MT, middle temporal), is a region of extra-striate visual cortex that is thought to play a major role in the perception of motion.

V5 is part of the broader "magno-cellular -- large cell -- system" that processes fast-moving objects, and brightness contrasts. One interpretation is that a specific magno-cellular cell type develops abnormally in people with dyslexia (3).

For results of clinical tests see (1)


Phoneme Processing

Main research from Shaymitz (1998) and Rumsy (1996), (see 5, 10,11, 14 –17) The language regions in the superior temporal gyrus (Wernike's area) and striate cortex are found to underachieve in the dyslexic. These areas respond to simple phoneme processing tasks. (Areas that respond to more complex language tasks, an anterior region, the IFG, displayed relative over activation in dyslexics.)

Games involving nonsense words, rhyme, and sound manipulation will be enhanced by special auditory effects: The consonants are recorded louder while the adjacent vowel is lengthened and its sounds softened. All games are carefully leveled by the complexity of the manipulations involved. (For results of clinical tests see Ojemann 1989, Bertoncini et al 1989 ).

Cross-modal Association

Main research from Leon (1996) and Shaymitz (1998) (see 8, 22, 24, 28,30)

The angular gyrus, a brain region considered pivotal in carrying out cross-modal (e.g., vision and language) associations necessary for reading, is involved. The current findings of under activation in the angular gyrus of dyslexic readers coincide with earlier studies of those who lost the ability to read due to brain damage centered in that same area of the brain.

The ability to link visual stimuli to auditory interpretation can be stimulated by Multimedia implementation of the coming together of these separate disciplines. Activities are all carefully leveled to correlate the child's current ability level.

Visual Recognition Skills

(Main school of research Livingstone (1993) and Martin and Lovegrove 1988) )(see 9, 15) Dyslexics have reduced synaptic activity in the V5 area.

V5 is part of the broader "magno-cellular -- large cell -- system" that processes fast-moving objects, and brightness contrasts. One interpretation is that a specific magno-cellular cell type develops abnormally in people with dyslexia (3).

For results of clinical tests see (1)

Working Memory

(Main school of research Beneventi et al.2008)

Reduced activity in the pre-frontal and parietal cortex may result in working memory deficits. #40

Symptoms

Common symptoms are:

  • Reading is typically slow and laborious. If they are undiagnosed or diagnosed late, they may be illiterate or barely literate.
  • Concentration tends to fluctuate.
  • Poor and unusual spelling and grammar. Handwriting is unusable or very messy.
  • Poor physical coordination
  • Difficulty remembering information, (tends to fluctuate)
  • Difficulty with organizing and planning
  • Difficulty working within time limits
  • Difficulty thinking and working in sequences, which can make planning difficult
  • Visual processing difficulties, which can affect reading and recognizing places
  • Poor auditory processing skills, which can make listening to oral instructions difficult, tiring and confusing

Challenges

Memory.

  • Poor short term memory for facts, events, times, dates, symbols.
  • Poor working memory; i.e. difficulty holding on to several pieces of information at the same time. This is especially challenging while undertaking a task e.g. taking notes as you listen, addressing compound questions.
  • Mistakes with routine information e.g. giving your age, and phone number or the ages of children.
  • Inability to hold on to information without referring to notes.

Automatizing Skills.

Dyslexics do not tend to automatize skills very well, and a high degree of mental effort is required in carrying out tasks that non-dyslexic individuals generally do not feel requires effort. This is particularly true when the skill is composed of several sub-skills (e.g. reading, writing, driving).

Information Processing.

  • Difficulties with taking in information efficiently (this could be written or auditory).
  • Slow speed of information processing, such as a 'penny dropping' delay between hearing or reading something and understanding and responding to it.

Communication Skills

  • Lack of verbal fluency and lack of precision in speech. (relevant for voice systems)
  • Word-finding problems
  • Inability to work out what to say quickly enough
  • Misunderstandings or misinterpretations during oral exchanges
  • Sometimes mispronunciations or a speech impediment may be evident.

Literacy

  • Difficulty in acquiring reading and writing skills. Reading is likely to be slow.
  • If they are undiagnosed or diagnosed late, may be illiterate, barely literate and it will be very laborious
  • Where literacy has been mastered, problems continue such as poor spelling, difficulty extracting the meaning from written material, difficulty with unfamiliar words, and difficulty with scanning or skimming text.
  • Particular difficulty with unfamiliar or new language such as jargon.

Organization, Sequencing

  • Difficulty organizing a sequence of events.
  • Incorrect sequencing of strings of numbers and letters. (passwords, phone numbers)
  • Chronic disorganization and misplacing/losing items.
  • Difficulty with time management and passage of time

Navigation

  • Difficulty with finding the way to places or navigating - even in the context of a building. Often get lost.

Sensory Sensitivity.

  • Sensitivity to noise and visual stimuli.
  • Impaired ability to screen out background noise / movement.
  • Sensations of mental overload
  • Tendency to "switch off".

Lack of Awareness.

  • Failure to notice body language.
  • Failure to realize the consequences of their speech or actions.

Visual Stress.

  • Some people with dyslexic difficulties may experience visual stress when reading but especially when dealing with large amounts of text. So breaks are often needed.

Coping Strategies

It must be emphasized that individuals vary greatly in their learning difficulties. Key variables are the severity of the difficulties and the ability of the individual to identify and understand their difficulties and successfully develop and implement coping strategies.

By adulthood, many people with dyslexia are able to compensate through technology, reliance on others and an array of self-help mechanisms - the operation of which require sustained effort and energy. Unfortunately, these strategies are prone to break down under stressful conditions which impinge on areas of weakness.

Effects of Stress.

People are particularly susceptible to stress (compared with the ordinary population) with the result that their impairments increase.

Scenarios and User Stories

Scenario: Online Research

A is a high school student with dyslexia. Although he can read his level is slow and he finds it difficult. A has a school project and needs to do online research. A does not use a screen reader as they are afraid that that will stop him reading and improving his skills. A needs to be able to find the content he needs easily, both finding the right resource and the right information inside that resource with minimal reading, and will then read the sections that he needs. He will do a web search, and a quick review of different pages to find the pages he needs.


Table of ICT Steps and challenges
Step Challenge
Search query
Scanning results
Doing a short review of different options and finding the most appropriate
Finding the right content in the right document
Reading the right content
Collecting the information
Copying for Citing the resources and collecting them with the right information
Saving the work
Putting it together and writing the paper Out of scope of this use case

Scenario: Finding Out About a Change Event in an Email

B is a mother with young children. She has dyslexia. B reads the words, and then stops to understand them. B is also a slow reader. B receives many emails and important emails often are below or behind the scroll bar. Reading the summaries of each email takes time. B has set her email app to tag emails from her child's school as important. However B still needs to differentiate between emails from her children’s school that are crucial and emails that are just informative. B needs to be able to find the important content (such as school finishing at a different time next Monday) in a long school newsletter.

B’s email application changes and she no longer knows how to tag senders as important. At the same time her child starts at a new school. She has difficulty finding the information on how to tag emails from the new school as important. Also the school starts sending many emails about projects they are doing and what is happening in class, so she does not have time to read each email from them as soon as it arrives. She postpones this task and important emails get lost.

Table of ICT Steps and challenges
Step Challenge
Finding out how to tag/label this from a sender as important (first time)
Remembering the process (re-finding it next time)
Tagging/labeling the new teacher
Identifying important emails from the teacher and distinguishing them from general interest emails
Finding important content in long emails


Scenario: Using a Electronic Interface

C is an adult living alone. He has dyslexia. C has impaired vision and auditory memory, and finds remembering sequences extremely challenging. C has a garden with an automatic watering system with a one line ICT (electronic) interface. The interface is not user friendly. C needs to select what sprinkler he is setting using an arrow key, then needs to set the first time it should go on, (using the arrow key in the number mode) press enter, and then set the duration the sprinkler should run. He then needs to repeat the steps for the second time (or leave it blank). He then needs to repeat the process for the next sprinkler in the correct order. C has been shown how to use the system many times, however each time the system needs to be adjusted he makes mistakes and gets confused. Ten years later C still needs to call the gardener to change the settings and is consonantly relearning the interface.

Table of ICT Steps and challenges
Step Challenge
learning the steps involved Learning the sequence
Performing the steps correctly Remembering the sequence. Performing it in the correct order
Undoing mistakes Remembering what point he is at in the sequence. Going back a step and tracking the step he is at now. -


Scenario: Using a Phone Menu

D is looking after his elderly father. D has dyslexia and impaired working memory and impaired auditory discrimination. D can do one mental process at a time. D is weak at remembering numbers. He can remember one number at a time. D typically makes mistakes when dialing numbers. Often he will dial a number 3 or 4 times before he gets it right. D needs to speak to a doctor about his father who is sick. The doctor's office has an answer phone system with multi-layers. It takes D two attempts to dial the office. When faced with the menu system, D needs to listen to several similar options, understand the words, process the words, make a choice and identify the correct number and enter the correct number into the keypad. Because he is trying to remember numbers whilst he is trying to listen to the next option he misunderstands the options. He makes an incorrect choice. When trying to recover from the error he enters an invalid number and gets thrown off the line. D needs to redial this number, but as he is now upset it takes him four attempts to dial it correctly. He is then faced with the same phone system. D makes more mistakes. After half an hour he asks a neighbor to help him. D is very upset which in turn upsets his sick father. D's self confidence at being able to look after his father is shattered.

Table of ICT Steps and challenges
Step Challenge
Identifying the option he needs and remembering the right number associated with that option. Auditory discrimination under pressure, memory of the correct number, whilst listening and processing other options.
Entering the correct number Mapping the symbol to the number under pressure, eye hand coordination
Undoing mistakes Staying calm so that his skills do not further deteriorate. -

How They Use The Web and ICT.

Dyslexics tend to use mainstream technologies to help them. For example, using a Word-processor's spell checker. They may use free screen readers or screen readers that highlight text as they read. They may use assistive technology such as Dragon or a Daisy reader, although this seems to be used more as a teaching aid and not for typical Web access. Special software to help dyslexics includes Text Help.

Characteristics of Content Optimized For This Group

Content made for people with dyslexia tends to have

  • Icons to visually reinforce structure and what each section is (such as examples, tips etc)
  • Diagrams that illustrate the point of the content
  • Short paragraphs, short sentences
  • Content tends to start with a summary of the point. This can also be true at the document or paragraph level where the first sentence raises the main point of the paragraph
  • Well structured text with headings (reducing reading of irrelevant text)
  • Use of bold on key terms (helps finding of relevant text)
  • A "read it to me" button, that highlights text as it is read and is simple to use
  • Has a clear, well-structured, minimalistic navigation system and is free from confusing steps and complex user interface flow

In general, content for dyslexics helps the user find the text they are looking for via visual aids, and reduces the need to read though irrelevant text to find the information that they are looking for.


Specific Technologies

Assistive technologies include (incomplete list):

  • Text help
  • Dragon
  • Kurzweil
  • Ghotit
  • Learning Ally
  • Zoomreader
  • speak-it
  • Read2Go


Summary of Existing Research and Guidelines

There are organizations who have produced guidelines for creating content for people with dyslexia, such as The British Dyslexia Association guidelines, and The Irish Dyslexia Association

Summary/Exerts of The British Dyslexia Association Guidelines and Dyslexia Style Guide

This Guide is in three parts: 1. Dyslexia Friendly Text. 2. Accessible Formats. 3. Website design.

1. Dyslexia Friendly Text

The aim is to ensure that written material takes into account the visual stress experienced by some dyslexic people, and to facilitate ease of reading. Adopting best practice for dyslexic readers has the advantage of making documents easier on the eye for everyone.

Media.

  • Use a plain, evenly spaced sans-serif font such as Arial. See the BDA New Technologies Committee website: http://bdatech.org/what-technology/typefaces-for-dyslexia/
  • Font size should be 12-14 point. Text should be expandable.
  • Use dark colored text on a light (not white) background. (Avoid pure white backgrounds because of glare)


Headings and Emphasis Headings

  • For Headings, use larger font size in bold, lower case.
  • Boxes and borders can be used for effective emphasis.
  • Avoid underlining and italics: these tend to make the text appear to run together. Use bold instead.
  • AVOID TEXT IN BLOCK CAPITALS: it is much harder to read.

Layout

  • Use left-justified with ragged right edge.
  • Avoid narrow columns (as used in newspapers).
  • Lines should not be too long: 60 to 70 characters.
  • Avoid cramping material and using long, dense paragraphs: space it out.
  • Line spacing of 1.5 is preferable.
  • Avoid starting a sentence at the end of a line.
  • Use bullet points and numbering rather than continuous prose.

Writing Style.

  • Use short, simple sentences in a direct style.
  • Give instructions clearly. Avoid long sentences of explanation.
  • Use active rather than passive voice.
  • Avoid double negatives.
  • Be concise.

2. Increasing Accessibility.

  • Flow charts are ideal for explaining procedures.
  • Pictograms and graphics help to locate information.
  • Lists of 'do's and 'don'ts' are more useful than continuous text to highlight aspects of good practice.
  • Avoid abbreviations if possible or provide a glossary of abbreviations and jargon.
  • For long documents include a contents page at the beginning and an index at end.

3. Checking Readability.

Note: You can set spell checker in MS Word to automatically check readability. MS Word will then show your readability score every time you spell check.

  • Check long documents in sections, so that you know which parts are too hard.
  • Flesch Reading Ease score: Rates text on a 100-point scale; the higher the score, the easier it is to understand the document. For most standard documents, aim for a score of approximately 70 to 80.


  • Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level score: Rates text on a U.S. grade-school level. For example, a score of 5.0 means that a fifth grader, i.e. a Year 6, average 10 year old, can understand the document. For most standard documents, aim for a score of approximately 5.0, by using short sentences, not by dumbing-down vocabulary.

Accessible Formats

Use an accessible format so that content can be read by screen reading software.

  • We suggest offering both the source MS Word files and derived PDF files where possible.
  • Publicise availability of accessible formats.

Preparing a Document for Text-Reading Software

  • Listening to a document using a text reader will take longer than visual reading.
  • Put full stops after headings to make the voice drop and pause; a pale tint similar to the background color will make the dots less visually distracting.
  • Put semi-colons, commas, or full stops after bullet points to make a pause.
  • Use Styles in Word to organize headings and formatting.
  • Contents Page listings should be hyper-linked.
  • Number menu items.
  • Use internal and external hyperlinks for ease of navigation.
  • Avoid text in capital letters in mid-line, as they may be read as single letters.
  • Include as few signs and symbols as are absolutely necessary, e.g. * asterisks or dashes (both short and long), as these will be spoken.
  • Long dashes should be avoided: use colons to make the voice pause.
  • Use straight quotation marks. Curly or slanting ones may be read out as ‘back quote’ by some screen readers.
  • Avoid Roman Numerals and 'No.' for number.
  • Consider whether abbreviations and acronyms need full stops.
  • Use hyphens in compound words to aid text reading pronunciation.
  • Chunk phone numbers to avoid being read as millions or hundreds of thousands.

Website Design

Research shows that readers access text at a 25% slower rate on a computer. This should be taken into account when putting information on the web. When a website is completed, check the site and information for accessibility by carrying out these simple checks.

  • Navigation should be easy. A site map is helpful.
  • Use graphics, images, and pictures to break up text, while bearing in mind that graphics and tables may take a long time to download.
  • Very large graphics make pages harder to read.
  • Offer alternate download pages in a text reader friendly style.
  • Where possible, design web pages which can be downloaded and read off-line.
  • Moving text creates problems for people with visual difficulties. Text reading software is unable to read moving text.
  • Content's links should show which pages have been accessed.
  • Encourage the use of hyperlinks at the end of sentences.
  • Make sure that it is possible for users to set their own choice of font style and size, background and print colours.

See Also

Other Guidelines

Tips found across the web include http://www.dyslexia.com/library/webdesign.htm

  • Keep paragraphs short, and use a small amount of text on each page.
  • If a long article is posted, create a topic index at the beginning, so that the dyslexic reader can quickly narrow in on the parts that interest him or her.
  • Use default font settings or provide a way for users to choose their own styles.

Read more: http://www.dyslexia.com/library/webdesign.htm#ixzz2yAl09G77

  • Use small icons to help with navigation between frequently used web pages.
  • Avoid using background images behind text. Make sure that there is a good contrast between the color of the background and the color of the text.
  • Do not set up background music to play, unless the site gives the user a choice whether to turn it on.

Read more: http://www.dyslexia.com/library/webdesign.htm#ixzz2yAjpgQlL

Extent To Which Current Needs Are Met

WCAG does help in that content can be used by a screen reader and headings should be used. Many of the most useful checkpoints are AAA and hence not implemented or are advisory techniques and hence, likewise, not adopted.

AA level conformance to WCAG does not significantly help reduce cognitive load or reduce dependency on text by formatting and pictorial aids. Other guidelines(non W3C such as British Dyslexia Association Guidelines) fill in some of the gaps in WCAG.

None of the reviewed guidelines help ICT interfaces of voice mail systems. They also do not address getting additional help.


Potentials and Possibilities

Added to brainstorming section

Prevalence

Dyslexia is a hidden disability thought to affect around 10% of the population, 4% severely.

Note that recent studies indicate that dyslexia is particularly prevalent among small business owners, with roughly 20 to 35 percent of US and British entrepreneurs being affected. This is important as often people feel the dyslexics are not in their user audience. With the exception of a scrabble game site, that is very unlikely. [39]

Sources and References

http://www2.open.ac.uk/students/disability/dyslexia-or-other-specific-learning-difficulties.php

http://www.bdadyslexia.org.uk/about-dyslexia/adults-and-business/dyslexia-and-specific-learning-difficulties-in-adu.html


References to Research

1. Bakker ,(1990) Neurophysiological Treatment of Dyslexia, Oxford University Press,.

2. A Galaburda,(1993) Dyslexia and Development: Neurobiological Aspects of Extra-Ordinary Brains, ed., Harvard University Press, London.

3. Getting the Message Across, published by the Questions Publishing Company, Birmingham, England, 1996, on behalf of the British Dyslexia Association.

4. B Blachman and L. Erlbaum.(1997) Foundations of Reading Acquisition and Dyslexia: Implications for Early Intervention, ed. Associates Publishers, New Jersey,.

5. S Shaywitz et all (1998) Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia . Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 95, pp. 2636–2641

6. C. Njioktien ,(1998) Nuerological Arguments for a joint developmental Dysphasia-Dyslexia syndrome

7. M Snowling (1997) Dyslexia A Cognitive Developmental Perrspective, Blackwell Massachusetts USA

8. G Lyon and j. Rumsey,(1996) Neuroimaging a window to the neurological foundations of learnng and behavior in children, Pual Brooks 58-73

9. Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods,R. P. & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996) Nature (London) 382, 66–69.

10. Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., Morton, J.,Frackowiak, R. S. J. & Frith, C. D. (1996) Brain 119, 143–157.

11. Rumsey, J. M., Nace, K., Donohue, B., Wise, D., Maisog, J. M.& Andreason, P. (1997) Arch. Neurol. 54, 562–573.

12. Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F. & Stone, G. O. (1990)Psychol. Rev. 97, 488–522.

13. Lukatela, G. & Turvey, M. T. (1994) J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 123,107–128.

14. Demonet, J. F., Price, C. Wise., R. & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1994)Brain 117, 671–682.

15. Henderson, V. W. (1986) Brain Lang. 29, 119–133.

16. Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Snyder, A. Z. & Raichle, M. E. (1990)Science 249, 1041–1044.

17. Pugh, K., Shaywitz, B., Constable, R. T., Shaywitz, S., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R., Bronen, R., Shankweiler, D., Katz, L., Fletcher,J. & Gore, J. (1996) Brain 119, 1221–1238.

18. Friston, K. J., Ashburner, J., Frith, C. D., Poline, J.-B., Heather,J. D. & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1995) Human Brain Mapping 2, 165–189.

19. Talairach, J. & Tournoux, P. (1988) Coplanar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Three-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging (Thieme, New York).

20. Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Jezzard, P., Padmanabhan, S., Clark, V. P., Karni, A., Prinster, A., Braun, A., Lalwani, A., Raus-checker, J. P. et al. (1997) J. Cognitive Neurosci. 9, 664–686.

21. Woodcock, R. W. (1987) Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests, Revised (American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MI).

22. Black, S. E. & Behrmann, M. (1994) in Localization and Neuro-imaging in Neuropsychology, ed. Kertesz, A. (Academic, New York), pp. 331–376.

23. Geschwind, N. (1965) Brain 88, 237–294.

24. Benson, D. F. (1994) The Neurology of Thinking (Oxford Univ. Press, New York).

25. Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitiz, F. & Geschwind, N. (1985) Ann. Neurol. 18, 222–233.

26. Geschwind, N. (1985) in Dyslexia: A Neuroscientific Approach to Clinical Evaluation, eds. Duffy, F. H. & Geschwind, N. (Little, Brown, Boston), pp. 195–211.

27. Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Constable, R. T., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R. K., Bronen, R. T., Fletcher, J. M., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L. et al. (1995) Nature (London) 373, 607–609.

28. Dejerine, J. (1891) C. R. Societe du Biologie 43, 197–201.

29. Damasio, A. R. & Damasio, H. (1983) Neurology 33, 1573–1583.

30. Friedman, R. F., Ween, J. E. & Albert, M. L. (1993) in Clinical Neuropsychology, eds. Heilman, K. M. & Valenstein, E. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York), pp. 37–62.

31. Benson, D. F. (1977) Arch. Neurol. 34, 327–331.

32. Tallal et all (1993) Temporal information processing in the nervous system: special reference to dyslexia and dysphasia. New York : The New York academy of science

33 Tallal et all (1998) New York : The New York academy of science

34. Hulme (1981) Reading and retardation and multisensory teaching: London :ROUTLEDGE AND KEGAN PAUL

35. Bradley (1981),

36, Thomson and Gilchrich: Dyslexia a multidisiplinary approch London Chapan and Hall

37. Bradley and Bryant (1983), Categorising sounds asnd learning to read. Nature, 301, 419.

38. Dyslexia at college Miles Dorothy and Gilrpoy 1986 New York Methuen

^ 39 Brent Bowers (2007-12-06). "Tracing Business Acumen to Dyslexia". New York Times.Cites a study by Julie Logan, professor of entrepreneurship at Cass Business School in London, among other literature.

40 b Berninger, Virginia W.; Raskind, Wendy, Richards, Todd, Abbott, Robert, Stock, Pat (5 November 2008). "A Multidisciplinary Approach to Understanding Developmental Dyslexia Within Working-Memory Architecture: Genotypes, Phenotypes, Brain, and Instruction". Developmental Neuropsychology 33 (6): 707–744. doi:10.1080/87565640802418662. PMID 19005912.

<a name="41">41</a> a b Stein, John (1 January 2001). "The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia". Dyslexia 7 (1): 12–36. doi:10.1002/dys.186.

42

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01684.x/abstract;jsessionid=0C68D8EFA292BDC2CD782A68E41AD2D4.f03t03

Cao, F., Bitan, T., Chou, T.-L., Burman, D. D. and Booth, J. R. (2006), Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47: 1041–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01684.x

43

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11305228

44

Developmental Psychopathology, Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation Donald J. Cohen John Wiley & Sons, Feb 27, 2006